However,...
My
last
posting on Garner's Rule -- which proscribes sentence-initial
linking
however -- ended with
an unresolved issue: I observed that college student writers seem to be
fond of the discourse connective
however,
and to prefer to put it in initial position (rather than
sentence-internally), and wondered why. That was Act I of
"The Story of
However".
Now, Act II, with some reasons. The zero-tolerance policy ZT-1,
"If they do it too much, they should be told not to do it at all", will
return to the stage and play a prominent role in this act.
(Acknowledgment: I'm reporting on joint work with Douglas Kenter.)
It's fairly easy to see why writers like discourse connectives (in
general, not just markers of contrast like
however and
but) in sentence-initial position:
if you use a marker C to connect a sentence S with preceding discourse
D according to the scheme
D C+S
the marker comes between the things whose contents it relates;
structure reflects function. In addition, sentence-initial
connectives are easy to produce and easy to process, while other
schemes of connection are more demanding. Sentence-internal
connectives are interruptions within their sentence:
The test is demanding.
Most students, however, will get all
the answers right.
Most students will, however, get all the answers right.
and sentence-final connectives hold off information about discourse
connection until the last possible moment, where it may come as
something of a surprise:
Most students will get all the answers
right, however.
The other main option, expressing discourse connection via a
subordinating conjunction on the sentence S' preceding S --
C+S' S
Although/Though the test is demanding, most students will get all the
answers right.
involves the complexity that is associated with subordination in
general.
The point here is not that these other options are inferior -- there
are occasions when they would be excellent choices -- but that a
sentence-initial linker is the simplest way to connect a sentence to
preceding discourse, so it's no surprise that students are inclined to
go for that scheme a lot of the time.
Ok, a sentence-initial connective, but which one? For expressing
contrast, the main contenders are
however and
but. These items differ in
(at least) three relevant ways: in their prosodic properties, in their
stylistic levels, and in their syntactic category. (Actually,
Kenter and I maintain that they also differ subtly in meaning and/or
discourse function, and we aren't the first to make this claim.
But that's a matter for another day.)
First, prosody.
However
has three syllables, has an accent of its own, and comes with a prosody
that separates it from the sentence it modifies.
But has only one syllable, is
usually unaccented, and is prosodically integrated with what
follows. Overall,
however
is a lot "weightier" prosodically than
but. Things follow from that.
Some people report that they like
however
just because it's more substantial, more prominent, than
but. They see
however as a more emphatic marker
of contrast, or at least a more noticeable one.
As I noted in my last posting, Bryan Garner sees things the other way;
he finds initial
however
"unemphatic". De gustibus and all that. But there are
reasons for a sensible person to like
but:
it's shorter and less ostentatious;
however
holds off the sentence that follows for an appreciable amount of time,
and it shouts "Contrast!" (As usual when we're comparing
alternatives, the things that distinguish them cut both ways,
functioning as either advantages or disadvantages, depending on the
context and the writer's purposes. That's why we should want both
alternatives to be available to writers and speakers.)
On to stylistic level. Here, people generally agree that
however is more formal than
but --
however groups with
adverbials like
moreover,
furthermore,
consequently,
therefore,
nevertheless,
and
nonetheless -- with the
result that many people like it when they're doing formal
writing. College students seem to like it especially, probably
because one of the things they're working at is to get the proper level
of formality in their writing. (They often overshoot, of course.)
(A little digression: complaints that initial
however is weak, monotonous, etc.
seem not to be extended to the other formal discourse adverbials in
initial position. The concentration on
however puzzles me;
furthermore is in competition with
and, and
consequently and
therefore with
so, in much the same way as
however with
but, yet
however gets all the
attention. Maybe it's just intellectual fashion. Maybe it's
all Strunk's fault.)
Notice that I said that
however
is more formal than
but, not
that
but is informal or
colloquial. My judgment here is that
but is in fact stylistically
neutral, usable at all levels, and this seems to be Garner's judgment
as well. In choosing between a neutral and a more formal
alternative, Garner seems to aim for a "plain style" and recommends the
neutral item, and in fact that's my practice too. That's why I
use so little sentence-initial
however.
(Garner's preference for neutral items over more formal alternatives
undoubtedly contributes to his enthusiasm for Fowler's Rule, insisting
on restrictive relative
that
over the more formal
which
when both are available.)
Finally, syntactic category. Here we approach the dramatic climax
of "The Story of
However".
However is an adverbial,
but a coordinating conjunction, and
this second fact introduces a conflict into our play's action.
A little story: whenever Kenter and I talk about our investigations
into
but and
however, a significant number of
people in our audiences are astounded to hear that there are
authorities actually
RECOMMENDING sentence-initial
but. Almost all of the
students in the audiences respond this way. (And now, after
yesterday's posting, my mailbox is filling up with similarly surprised
messages from all over the place.) But, but, they clamor, we were
taught
NEVER to begin a sentence with
but, or any other coordinating
conjunction (
and and
so are the other usual offenders).
Taught where? In grade school and high school. No Initial
Coordinators (NIC) is all over the place in those precincts. Some
Stanford undergraduates told us that their section instructors in PWR
(Program in Writing and Rhetoric, the successor to Freshman
Composition) insisted on NIC. I happen to know that the main
texts used in PWR do not advocate NIC, so these section instructors
were rolling their own advice (well, probably just handing on things
they themselves had been taught). Still, NIC had some college
presence. And at Stanford. I was appalled.
In any case, what were the kids taught in elementary and secondary
school? Don't use
but
to start a sentence;
USE HOWEVER
INSTEAD! So of course college students very frequently
opt for
however; it's just
what they were taught to do. Now we see the dramatic conflict:
NIC vs. Garner's Rule. You can't obey them both.
I will soon speculate on the origins of NIC. But first, some
disavowals of NIC, beginning with Mark Liberman right here
on Language
Log:
There is nothing in the grammar of the
English language to support a prescription against starting a sentence
with and or but --- nothing in the norms of
speaking and nothing in the usage of the best writers over the entire
history of the literary language. Like all languages, English is full
of mechanisms to promote coherence by linking a sentence with its
discourse context, and on any sensible evaluation, this is a Good
Thing. Whoever invented the rule against sentence-intitial and and but,
with its a preposterous justification in terms of an alleged defect in
sentential "completeness", must have had a tin ear and a dull mind.
Nevertheless, this stupid made-up rule has infected the culture so
thoroughly that 60% of the AHD's (sensible and well-educated) usage
panel accepts it to some degree.
(And, sadly, Microsoft's Grammar Checker tries to enforce NIC.)
Mark notes that the AHD note for
and
rejects NIC out of hand, and he provides a smorgasbord of cites (and
statistics) from reputable authors. Similarly MWDEU. Paul
Brians, collector of common errors in English, labels sentence-initial
coordinators a "non-error". Bryan Garner denies, all over the
place, that NIC has any validity. Even the curmudgeonly Robert
Hartwell Fiske tells his readers that there's absolutely nothing wrong
with sentence-initial coordinators. A point of usage and style on
which Liberman and I and the AHD and the MWDEU stand together with
Brians and Garner and Fiske (and dozens of other advice writers) is,
truly, not a disputed point. NIC is crap.
But still it lives on, as what I've called a
zombie
rule. It's been lurking in the grammatical shadows for some
time -- at
least a hundred years, to judge from MWDEU. Hardly any usage
manual subscribes to it, but it
is, apparently, widely taught in schools, at least in the U.S., with
the result that educated people tend to be nagged by a feeling that
there is something bad about sentence-initial
and (and
but and
so). (It might well be that
this sense of unease rises with level of education. Someone
should look at this possibility.)
I speculate now about two questions: how did the proscription arise,
and why does it persist?
Grammatical
proscriptions that are at odds with elite usage can arise in three
ways, two of which were probably at work in the case of
sentence-initial
and/but/so:
as an expression of individual taste; as a consequence of "theoretical"
claims about grammar; and as a by-product of well-intentioned efforts
to improve student writing and speech.
Most of the advice literature on English is the product of individual
people -- essayists, poets, editors, journalists, literary scholars,
lawyers, translators, and other people who deal in a practical way with
language -- who see themselves as serving as arbiters of style as well
as guardians of the formal standard written language. There's
plenty of room on matters of style for the arbiters to retail their
personal likes and dislikes as instructions to others. But as far
as I can tell, the impulse to impose personal taste has played no
significant role in the rise of the NIC
zombie.
But "theoretical" considerations surely have. There is a
widespread belief that sentences -- in both writing and speaking --
should be "complete", not fragmentary, in fact that complete
SENTENCES
are signs of "complete", well-ordered
THOUGHTS (and
that incomplete, fragmentary sentences are signs of incomplete,
disordered thoughts). The underpinning belief is that the
superficial syntactic form of sentences is a direct reflection of the
structure of the thoughts these sentences convey. This is a very
silly idea, and when it's combined with an almost exclusive attention
to single sentences, rather than organized discourses, it yields the
claim that fragmentary sentences are very bad things.
(Animosity towards fragmentary sentences has had occasionally
pernicious results -- perhaps, most famously, in claims by Bereiter and
Englemann back in the '60s that kids, or at least impoverished black
kids, who answered wh-questions (
Where
is the monkey?) with fragments rather than full sentences (
In the tree rather than
The monkey is in the tree) were
betraying an inability to think clearly. The recommended
treatment for their deficit in thinking was drilling on always
producing complete sentences in answers to questions.)
NIC
can
be seen as just a special case of No Fragmentary Sentences. The
function of conjunctions like
and,
but, and
so -- the only function of such
conjunctions, it is claimed -- is the joining of phrases of like type,
so that a sentence that begins with one of these words is missing the
clause that is to be joined with the clause that follows the
conjunction, and that sentence is therefore only fragmentary.
(Yes, I know, the clause is right there in the previous sentence, but
we're supposed to be looking only at single sentences here.) If
you take all the beliefs and claims above literally, you are led to the
conclusion that NIC
is not only true, but necessarily true.
But few advice manuals are willing to go all the way with this
"theoretical" argumentation. For example, Diana Hacker's
A Pocket Style Manual, 4th ed.
(Boston: Bedford/St. Martin's, 2004), p. 48, tells the student, "As a
rule, do not treat a piece of a sentence as if it were a sentence" and
goes on to classify fragments into two types: "Some fragments are
clauses that contain a subject and a verb but begin with a
subordinating word. Others are phrases that lack a subject, a
verb, or both."
Notice: "a
SUBordinating word". Hacker's rule
applies only to subordinate clauses. And, indeed, in the text
that follows there's a list of sample words that begin subordinate
clauses.
And,
but, and
so are not on this list; by
inference, they are allowed in sentence-initial position.
It's likely that the main justification for NIC comes instead from
well-intentioned
attempts to improve student writing and speaking. Initial
and is the first sentence
connective acquired by most English-speaking children, and they use it
heavily in their speech; of course they do, since for a while it's all
they've got for indicating connection between sentences. Heavy
use of sentence-initial
and
and (logical/temporal)
so
continues through childhood and into adulthood, in both speaking and
writing, with
then and
and then as additional variants in
narratives. Observe the discourse organization of the Coasters'
rousing "Along Came Jones", from about 45 years ago:
And then he grabbed her (And then)
He tied her up (And then)
He turned on the bandsaw (And then, and then...!)
And then along came Jones
Tall thin Jones
Slow walkin' Jones
Slow talkin' Jones
Along came long, lean, lanky Jones
Teachers quite rightly view this system of sentence connection as
insufficiently elaborated, and they seek ways of getting students to
produce connectives that have more content than vague association or
sequence in time. At some point, I speculate, they applied ZT-1,
"If they do it too much, they should be told not to do it at all", and
NIC, a blanket proscription, was born. Probably in elementary
schools, from which it would have diffused to secondary schools and
beyond. And now the zombie lurches on, possibly inside your own
computer; it's inside mine, thanks to Microsoft Word for Mac OS X.
Once NIC is out there, it will persist. Any fool with a claim to
authority and either students or a publisher can get a rule
ON
the books, but there is absolutely no mechanism for getting rules
OFF.
People think that rules are important, and they are reluctant to
abandon things they were taught as children, especially when those
teachings were framed as matters of right and wrong. They
will pass those teachings on. They will interpret denials
of the validity of such rules -- even denials coming from people like
Garner and Fiske, who are not at all shy about slinging rules around --
as threats to the moral order and will tend to reject them. I've
had some success convincing some students and friends that some of the
rules they were taught are not good rules to live by -- but my success
depends on their willingness to listen to me and their willingness to
question their beliefs, two qualities that are not widespread in the
general population.
So our little play goes: ZT-1 contributes significantly to the rise of
NIC and then Garner's Rule, though these originally have different
audiences. Eventually, the two proscriptions clash, and, in my
telling of the story, NIC is mortally wounded, but continues to wander
the landscape as a zombie. Garner's Rule survives, in a community
of like-minded souls pugnatiously defending themselves against the
opinions of linguists and the practices of many of the neighbors.
Nothing is ever resolved.
zwicky at-sign csli period stanford period edu
Posted by Arnold Zwicky at November 1, 2006 01:49 PM